Children learn from the language they hear. Growing up in a rich language environment can support children’s oral language development and lay the foundations for the emergence of literacy. Before children start school, much of the language they hear comes from caregivers and people in their immediate environment, but everyday talk is not the only type of language children might encounter. Often caregivers will read to their children, and this offers many potential benefits for children’s learning and development. One reason why reading to children might be so important is because the language of books is very different to the language of spoken communication.
The first part of our project involved finding out how 'book language' differs from the language children hear via day-to-day conversation. To do this, we analysed large datasets consisting of child-directed speech (records of caregivers speaking to 0-6 year-olds) and child-directed text, that is, the language content of children's books written for young children to hear in the context of shared reading. We then compared the two types of language input, focussing on both lexical and syntactic factors.
Our research shows that even picture books targeted at very young children contain a wider range of words, which also tend to be more abstract and sophisticated, compared to child-directed speech. What’s more, we found that book language provides access to types of sentence structures that are rarely encountered in speech, but which are important for children’s own developing literacy skills. This means that reading (or being read to) provides children with the opportunity to experience language that they otherwise wouldn't encounter.Â
Some of the differences between children's book language and conversational language are summarised in the infographic below. In our recent review paper, we describe and discuss these differences, and consider the implications of 'book language' for children's language, literacy and emotional development.Â